Research
in Education
An
Introduction
Research
in Education
An
Introduction
Billy L. Turney
George P. Robb
North Texas State University
The Dryden Press Inc.
Hinsdale, Illinois
Copyright @ 1971 by The Dryden Press
Inc.
All rights reserved
Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: 79-154269
ISBN: 0-03-089004-7 Cloth
ISBN: 0-03-089117-5 Paper
Printed in the United States of America
789 090 9876
Preface
This textbook has been designed
and written primarily for use by the student who is taking his
first course in educational research. In the development of the
book we have assumed that the reader could be either a consumer
or a potential producer of research, and we have striven to
achieve what we believe is a proper balance of research theory
and application. Although attention has been focused upon the
student who is experiencing his initial formal contact with
research methodology, we have not been unmindful of the fact that
others might profit from use of the book. It may well serve as a
useful reference for classroom teachers, counselors, or
administrators who are interested in doing research in their
schools but need a "refresher course."
As an introductory text, this
book is, of necessity, limited in scope and depth. We feel,
however, that no important areas of research are omitted as far
as the beginning student is concerned. The language used
throughout the book is reasonably nontechnical and concise.
Numerous examples and illustrations have been drawn from the
field of education in order to clarify important points and to
enhance the reader's understanding of the essential principles,
concepts, and techniques of research.
The sequence of the first five
chapters roughly follows the steps that a researcher might be
expected to take in conducting a study. Chapter I provides an
introduction to research and stresses the importance of the
spirit of scientific inquiry in research. Chapter 2 deals with
the selection and evaluation of problems for research. Chapter 3
explains the essential features of a research proposal and the
procedures for writing one. Chapters 4 and 5 concern the use of
the library
vii
viii PREFACE
and the review of related literature, which are very important
aspects of the research process.
The next five chapters discuss
the characteristics of three common types of research, the
processing of research data, statistical analysis of data,
research bias, and various tools and techniques of data
gathering. The reporting of educational research, which marks the
final stage in the research process, is discussed in Chapter 11.
This chapter is sufficiently detailed to suffice as a form book
for writing a research report. Thus, the use of this textbook may
obviate the need to acquire a separate manual in a research
course. Finally, samples of a research proposal, research report,
and abstract are presented in the last chapter. These samples are
used simply as illustrations of how a completed proposal, report,
and abstract might look rather than as models for a research
project. The accompanying critiques also should be helpful to the
student.
Several of the chapters have been
edited from manuscript submitted by various professors of
education and psychology at North Texas State University. We are
indebted to these individuals for the use of their material and
feel that their contributions are of inestimable value. The
contributing authors are: R. C. Bradley, (Chapter 1), R. T Hinely
(Chapter 2), Charles May, now at Indiana State University
(Chapter 3), Benjamin Coody (Chapter 5), James W Turner and John
Curry (Chapter 6), Earl Kooker (Chapter 8), Earl McCallon
(Chapter 10), and John Plunkett (Chapter 11).
We gratefully acknowledge the
assistance of Gwendel Mulkey, Tarrant County junior College, in
the preparation of the samples of the research proposal and
report found in Chapter 12. The critiques of these samples were
done most capably by Linda Mitchell Crocker, University of
Florida. We are also appreciative of the valuable editorial and
production assistance rendered by Roger Williams, Robert
Ballinger, and Donna Conte of The Dryden Press.
We are indebted to the Literary Executor of the late Sir Ronald
A. Fisher, F.R.S., to Dr. Frank Yates, F.R.S., and to Oliver and
Boyd Ltd., Edinburgh, for permission to reprint Tables III and VI
from their book Statistical Tables for Biological,
Agricultural, and Medical Research, 6th ed., 1963.
It is hoped that the use of this
textbook will go beyond providing an understanding of the methods
and techniques of research, as important as this objective may
be. We hope that the book will move students and educators toward
a greater awareness of the value of the research process as a
means of effecting improvement in education.
February
1971 Denton, Texas | | B. L. T. G. P. R. |
Contents
Prefacevii
1.Introduction 1
What Constitutes Research 2
The Scientific Approach to
Research 3
Types of Research 7
Assessment of Research Potential
8
2. Selection and
Evalution of a Problem11
Problem Origins11
Evaluation of a Problem16
3.The Research Proposal
21
Preparing the Research Proposal22
Procedures in Preparing Research Proposals22
4.The Use of the Library
in Educational Research33
Books33
Periodicals40
Abstracts43
Newspapers44
Pamphlets44
Government Documents44
Educational Resources Information Center46
5.A Review of Related
Literature49
Selection of Studies To Be Reported49
ix
xCONTENTS
Procedures for Reviewing the Literature51
Preparation of the Related Literature Report55
6.Types of
Research59
Historical Research59
Descriptive Research62
Experimental Research65
7.Analysis and
Treatment of Data71
Organization of Data71
Graphical Representation of Data73
Form of a Frequency Distribution76
Percentiles76
Computing Percentile Ranks78
Computing Percentiles79
Measures of Central Tendency80
Selection of Measures of Central Tendency81
Measures of Variability82
Computing Standard Scores85
Normal Frequency Distribution Curve86
Inferential Statistics88
Random Sampling89
Central Limit Theorem90
Confidence Intervals and Levels of Confidence90
Significance of Differences between Means92
Correlation95
Product-Moment Coefficient of Correlation97
Interpretation of a Correlation Coefficient99
Equation of a Regression Line100
Spearman Rank-Order Correlation101
8.Factors Affecting
Research Results105
Sample Selection107
Measurement of Variables109
Variables Affecting Both Field Studies and
Experiments112
Variables Affecting Field Studies114
Variables Affecting a Field Experiment116
Use of Control and Experimental Groups118
Statistical Tests of Hypotheses122
Questions Regarding Experimentation125
Application of Research as Related to Design126
CONTENTSxi
9.Tools and Techniques
of Research129
Methods of Questioning130
Rating Scales and Methods135
The Observation Method142
Sociometric Techniques144
Tests and Inventories150
Basic Characteristics of Measurement Instruments154
10.Computational Aids
for the Researcher161
Desk Calculators161
Electronic Digital Computer163
The Stored Program170
11.Reporting
Educational Research173
General Content of the Report173
Style and Format177
Footnote Citations182
Tables186
Illustrations188
Bibliography188
Abstract189
Sample Pages 191
12.A Research Study
Example221
Sample Research Proposal223
Critique of Sample Research Proposal241
Sample Research Report246
Critique of Sample Research Report295
Sample of an Abstract301
Critique of Sample Abstract304
Appendix305
Table A307
Table B308
Table C309
Table D310
Table E311
Index313
1
Introduction
Many of the educational practices used in schools today have
grown out of activities that resemble research techniques.
Undoubtedly, the history of the growth of these practices would
show that they were found to be sound through chance, trial-and-
error, and practical experience. Hence, as educators began to
reason and apply information, to repeat and perfect methods of
instruction, new facts and ideas not previously known were
obtained. From these grass roots have sprung the many facets of
growth in the educator's present knowledge of research.
All educators can benefit from
the knowledge of the nature, techniques, and procedures of
scientific research. In the first place, this knowledge is useful
because these skills will enable classroom teachers and other
professionals to utilize scientific methods in attacking their
own practical problems. Secondly, research knowledge is essential
because it can furnish school personnel with the information
necessary to make objective decisions concerning curriculum,
methods, administrative procedures, and so on. Thirdly, knowledge
of research permits the educator to be both a consumer and
producer of research. It seems logical to assume that
those who borrow from the research reservoir will desire to
contribute to the ever increasing supply of valid knowledge of
education through research.
School personnel are in a key
position to study the effects of new materials, methodology, and
similar innovations on the learning process. Research that
actively involves the educator in a problem that has meaning to
him, insofar as the results can be directly applied to his
teaching or administrative position, can contribute substantially
to improvement of the educational process.
1
2INTRODUCTION
WHAT CONSTITUTES RESEARCH?
Through the ages learned men have sought to solve problems of
society scientifically. A succession of scientists have been the
pathfinders and lamplighters to our nation's progress. As a
result of their research efforts man lives longer, enjoys more
leisure, and has greater use of intellectual power than ever
before.
The chief purposes for conducting
research are: (1) to determine the status of phenomena (past and
present); (2) to ascertain the nature, composition, and processes
that characterize selected phenomena; (3) to trace growth,
developmental history, change, and status of certain phenomena-,
and (4) to study the cause-and-effect relationships among and
between certain phenomena.
Although man has not yet devised
any perfect method of finding solutions to problems deemed worthy
of investigation, progress has been made. There has been a
gradual transition from seeking knowledge based purely on custom,
tradition, authority, and personal experience, to appealing for
evidence based on reasoning and scientific inquiry.
As Hillway states:
No longer does man ascribe natural phenomena to
supernatural influences, and no longer does he rely blindly upon
accepted authority. He has developed an orderly system of
searching for truth which, by basing conclusions upon factual
evidence and by using logic as a means of showing relationships
between related ideas, has given him better and more accurate
answers to his many questions. This orderly system is what we
call research.1
A more thorough understanding of
what constitutes research may be obtained by examining
experimental problems. McCall states that experimental problems
can best be identified:
(1) by becoming a scholar in one or more specialties
as early as possible; (2) by reading, listening, and working
critically and reflectively; (3) by considering every obstacle an
opportunity for the exercise of ingenuity instead of an
insuperable barrier; (4) by starting an investigation and
watching problems bud from it; and (5) by remembering those
problems already found, that is, by keeping a systematic record
of original ideas and problems.2
Thus, research is a formal,
systematic, intensive process used in the investigation of a
problem. In the educational realm, it may be carried on by an
individual, team, or organization. It may be conducted in a
class, school, or community. Research is not limited to a
laboratory setting.
1 Tyrus Hillway, Introduction to
Research (Cambridge, Mass.: Houghton Mifflin, 1956), p.
18.
2 W A. McCall, How to Experiment in Education
(New York: Macmillan, 1925), pp. 7, 8.
THE SCIENTIFIC APPROACH TO RESEARCH 3
THE SCIENTIFIC APPROACH TO
RESEARCH
Generally, the quest for truth takes a person to one of five
major sources of evidence: (1) tradition; (2) learned authority;
(3) personal experience; (4) logical reasoning; and (5)
scientific investigation. A review of the history of the development of research technology will
reveal that these sources are
listed here chronologically. Because the last two sources offer
the best prospects for new knowledge, understanding, and insight,
the discussion here will be limited to "scientific inquiry."
The scientific method involves a
double movement of thought. The investigator directs his
attention from the partially known and oftentimes confused
information learned from observation, previous investigations,
reflective thinking, and so on, toward a meaningful whole
or generalization. Secondly, he moves back from this
suggested whole or generalization to the particular parts in
order to connect these with one another in a meaningful pattern.
The first of these movements is inductive; the second,
deductive.3 The process of thinking is considered
complete when the investigator moves to and from a meaning, with
appropriate interaction of his reasoning processes occurring
between the recollected consideration and far-reaching, general
meanings. As Dewey succinctly put it:
While induction moves from fragmentary details (or
particulars) to a connected view of a situation (universal),
deduction begins with the latter and works back again to
particulars, connecting them and binding them
together.4
On the one hand induction is a movement toward the discovery of
some binding principle, while on the other hand deduction is a
movement toward the testing of this principle (for example,
confirming, accepting, modifying). Insofar as the investigator is
able to interpret isolated details and see them in the light of
this organizing principle, he will find valid relationships.
Application of the Scientific
Method
There is no special point at which an investigator declares he is
using the scientific method. Nevertheless, he applies the
scientific method in the beginning stages of the selection of his
problem. He weighs evidence with respect to the problem to be
studied and views its possibilities from several vantage points.
He seeks answers to the following types of questions: "What does
related research show?" "To what extent is this problem
researchable?" "Can the problem be solved with present technical
tools?" "Is it worth the time and effort essential to its
ultimate exploration?" and "To what extent does this problem
anchor itself in
3John Dewey, How We Think
(Boston: D. C. Heath, 1911), pp. 79-100.
4John Dewey, How We Think, p. 87.
4INTRODUCTION
the already established laws and principles?" Unless the problem
to be studied is expressed in some tentative form so that it can
be thoroughly analyzed for revision purposes, it may never move
from an emotional level to an intellectual statement worthy of
investigation. Likewise, if the investigator does not use the
procedures of scientific inquiry for the purpose of feeding
information from established laws and principles back to his
original theoretical problem, it is doubtful that it will be
altered as dictated by the evidence.
In employing the use of the
scientific method, the investigator starts with a
hypothesis5 as a guide for determining what
type of data to gather. On the basis of an analysis of the
collected data, the hypothesis is accepted, modified, or refuted.
It should be pointed out here that sometimes the hypothesis will
not be stated in its final form until some of the facts are
available for examination. Although the investigator's thoughts
move back and forth among the following steps in the process of
analysis of the problem, there is a logical sequence to the
elements that comprise the scientific method in education:
Step 1. |
The
investigator identifies and defines the problem. |
Step 2. | He
formulates a testable hypothesis. |
Step 3. | He collects,
organizes, tabulates, and analyzes his data. |
Step 4. | He
formulates conclusions on the basis of his findings. |
Step 5. | He
appraises these new conclusions in the light of future needs (educational
implications). |
Skillfully employed, these steps will help an investigator reach his objectives.
Inductive Reasoning
The inductive method is essentially the method of discovery. It
moves from objects or keynote examples to the development of
ideas. Generally it is believed that conclusions reached by
deductive reasoning are true only if derived from tenable
premises. Consequently, man has searched for a more thorough way
of determining whether his observations arc justified. Inductive
reasoning seems to be the answer to his quest, although it cannot
be relied upon exclusively. Inductive reasoning, therefore, has
been devised to complement deductive reasoning.
The investigator who collects
information about respondents, conditions, or behaviors of a
related group, may do so in order to establish generalizations
relative to a larger group. This would be called imperfect
induction. For example, to determine the number of children
in a school who need to be assigned to a remedial reading class,
the investigator can use each child in the entire school in his
investigation. Since it is not always practical to examine all
the instances
5 A tentative theory or supposition
adopted for the purpose of explaining certain known conditions
and providing a guide in the research process.
THE SCIENTIFIC APPROACH TO RESEARCH 5
that might be considered in arriving at a generalization
regarding a given group or class, the investigator wisely seeks
to examine a smaller representative group.6 His findings might
then be generalized to the total group represented by this
smaller group.
If the size and representation of
the sample are adequate, some inferences probably can be made
with respect to the total group. An investigator who uses
imperfect induction, however, must, recognize that some
unexamined instances of a particular class may not agree with his
conclusions, but where investigative procedures have been sound,
reliable information results.
Deductive Reasoning
In deduction, the investigator reasons that whatever is true of
all events in a group or class must also be true of any single
instance that comes within its domain. The principle of deduction
is: If A is true and B is true, then under certain
specified conditions one can infer that C is true. To
determine whether a particular instance or event under
consideration logically falls within this principle of deduction,
the investigator uses a device known to researchers as a
syllogism, which in deductive reasoning consists of a
major and a minor premise and a conclusion. A syllogism provides
the researcher with a means of testing the validity of a
particular conclusion. Definitive examples of four different
types of syllogisms follow.
An Alternative Syllogism | Either
the car
will be fixed or it will not go very far. (major premise)
The car has not been fixed. (minor premise)
Therefore, the car will break down soon. (conclusion) |
A Categorical Syllogism |
Men are mortal beings.
The President is a man.
Therefore, the President is mortal. |
A Disjunctive Syllogism |
It is not the case that it is both a snowy afternoon and a
pleasant afternoon for the football game.
It is a snowy day.
Therefore, it is not a good afternoon for football |
A Hypothetical Syllogism |
If the courthouse is on fire, the archives are in danger.
The courthouse is on fire.
Therefore, the archives are in danger. |
As shown in the examples above,
arguments may involve alternative, categorical, disjunctive, and
hypothetical propositions. It can be seen that each
6 This smaller group, which is a subset
of the total group, is called a sample. A more detailed
explanation of sample selection is found in Chapter 8.
6 INTRODUCTION
syllogism is labeled according to the type of proposition
occurring in the major premise. If a person accepts the first two
premises, he must also agree to the conclusion that would
logically follow.
One of the limitations of
deductive reasoning is the possibility of ambiguity, since
deduction relies on verbal symbols that may not have the same
meaning to every person. Another limitation is that one can only
work from existing knowledge, and still another limitation is the
possibility that the premise upon which a conclusion is to be
drawn may not be true. Certainly one can draw conclusions from
premises that have been declared valid, but what if the premises
are not in accordance with reality? Consider the following
example:
All research investigators hold master's degrees.
Mr. John Doe is a research investigator for a public school.
Therefore, Mr. John Doe holds a master's degree.
Based on the major premise, it would seem that Mr. Doe must hold
a master's degree, but in reality Mr. Doe only holds graduate
certification in his area of research. Hence, conclusions drawn
from deductive arguments can only be as trustworthy as the
premises from which they are derived.
The more the investigator uses
the scientific processes of induction and deduction for
regulating the conditions under which suggestions are allowed to
spring up and develop, the more likely he will guide his thinking
toward obtaining a working hypothesis. When care is taken to use
systematic thinking while moving toward a hypothesis and then
back again toward the facts, important connections will be seen.
The connections may become premises (grounds, foundations, bases)
and are said to support the conclusion. The movement toward
building up the research hypothesis is known as inductive
discovery and the movement toward developing, applying, and
testing, is known as deductive proof.
The Scientific Method of Analysis
Of the many distinguishing features of scientific analysis, six
characteristics seem most prominent: (1) Science is based on
fact(s); (2) Science employs the principles of analysis as a
fundamental procedure in the dealing with complex phenomena; (3)
Science employs the use of hypotheses in the thinking involved;
(4) Science promotes freedom from emotional bias; (5) Science
involves the use of accurate and concise measurement; and (6)
Science promotes systematized thinking. The scientific method has
one characteristic that no other method of attaining knowledge
has: self-correction. In elaborating upon the scientific approach
Kerlinger has stated:
There are built-in checks all along the
way to scientific knowledge. These checks are so conceived and
used that they control and verify the scientist's activities and
conclusions to the end of attaining dependable knowledge outside
himself. Even if a
TYPES OF RESEARCH
7
hypothesis seems to be supported in an
experiment, the scientist will test alternative hypotheses that,
if also supported, may cast doubt on the first
hypothesis.7
The research worker should not accept a given statement as true
merely because at first glance it may seem tenable. Neither
should self-imposed restrictions resulting from previous learning
experiences, nor admonitions given by self-ppointed experts who
decry, "This is the way these projects typically are done,"
become pressures to which the researcher succumbs. Any researcher
after accepting his assumptions is obligated to base his beliefs
on something; He must be able to explain the rationale behind his
hypotheses. He then sets out to test these hypotheses.
TYPES OF RESEARCH
It is customary to classify research as either applied
research or basic research. Applied research is
directed toward the solution of an immediate, specific, practical
problem. Basic research is concerned with the formulation
of a theory or a contribution to the existing body of
knowledge.
For example, If a researcher were
to conduct a study using white rats to determine the effect of
positive versus negative reinforcement on learning to run through
a maze successfully, he would be conducting basic research. If he
rewarded one group of rats with food pellets for correct turns in
the maze, but gave an electrical shock to the group of rats
making incorrect turns, he could then determine which group
mastered the maze in a shorter period of time. Even though this
information has no immediate application to motivation, it might
be useful in the building of a theory concerning motivation and
learning.
Whereas food and electrical shock
might be impractical to use in the classroom, the researcher
could conduct an experiment using praise and criticism as
motivational factors in human learning. This type of experiment
would be directed toward the solution of a practical problem and,
thus, would be classified as applied research.
In addition to the broader
classification of applied and basic research, nearly all studies
may be classified as historical, descriptive, or
experimental research. Their classification may be
determined by posing the following questions:
1. Does the research deal with
what once was? If it does, then it is historical
research. The educational historian may seek either to
produce an accurate description of unique events that have
happened in the past or to suggest through the survey of these
events fruitful generalizations from prior experiences that may
act as controls for behavior in the present or future. His
purpose is to produce
7 Fred N. Kerlinger, Foundations of
Behavioral Research (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston,
1965), pp. 7, 8.
8INTRODUCTION
evidence that will help us profit by the experiences of the past
in the solution of current problems.
2. Does the research deal with
what is? If it does, then it is descriptive research.
Descriptive research is that process that is concerned with
characterizing the features of situations, objects, or practices.
It allows one to find out pertinent information about an existing
situation. Descriptive research usually is thought of as an
effort to determine current practices or status so that we may
develop guidelines for future practices.
3. Does the research deal with
what can be when certain factors are controlled? If it does,
then quite likely some attempt is being made to establish cause
and effect relationships in a controlled situation.
Experimental research is an attempt to control all
essential factors with the exception of one or more independent
variables that can be manipulated, with the purpose of
determining and measuring the effect of their operation under
given circumstances. Once these effects are determined through
demonstration, then true control of behavior or the cnvironment
becomes possible.
These three types of research
will be discussed more extensively in Chapter 6.
ASSESSMENT OF RESEARCH POTENTIAL
Generally, one undertakes his first research project as a
graduate student in education. As the student assesses the
feasibility of undertaking a particular project, he becomes aware
of the brief time available for graduate study, the cost of
carrying on a comprehensive research project, the limited
professional training he has at the onset of the project, and the
mass of related research he must review. University and college
faculties may alleviate such difficulties by requiring students
to select their problems for research as soon as possible, by
providing more careful supervision to aid in the selection of a
research problem, by coordinating the projects of several
students that may overlap in objectives, methods, or subjects,
and by providing subsidies for important and desirable types of
research that otherwise would be beyond the budget of the typical
graduate student.
Though the facts mentioned above
are important and must be considered by all graduate students as
external variables with which research workers must deal, some
cffort must be channeled toward dealing with the six common
shortcomings of graduate research as observed by Symonds: (1)
Gathering of facts has been correlated inadequately with
application of these facts to the educational program; (2) The
background of information and understanding of the student is
often too mcager and shallow in the area in which he wishes to
explore ; (3) Fundamental assumptions often not explicitly
recognized and understood. give the study a particular bias and
make it of limited value, (4) Due to vested interests, certain
groups (companies, foundations, organizations) who provide the
graduate
ASSESSMENT OF RESEARCH POTENTIAL 9
student with money or material for research purposes, or both,
expect the completed study to cast their product in a more
favorable light; (5) The methods of investigation and the
selection of the instruments to be used are often faulty; and (6)
Technical faults in statistical methods selected are very common.
An awareness of these shortcomings, which ultimately enables the
investigator to avoid them, is a measure of research
scholarship.8
A producer of research is first a
consumer of research. He must be skilled in detecting bias,
unfounded conclusions, inaccuracies, and so on, or these faults
will appear in his own work. Those who write research papers
recognize that the reader is likely to read with a critical eye
and an informed mind. just as the classroom teacher grades his
daily papers to find out whether pupils have answered the
questions correctly, so does the professional evaluate research.
The reporter of research must anticipate the questions that will
arise in the mind of the analytical reader if his report is to be
regarded as a scholarly contribution.
An important attribute of a
researcher is the ability to be objective. He reports what he
finds irrespective of his feelings about the results obtained. He
reports all of what he finds. He does not present only the data
that support a hypothesis while at the same time suppressing,
camouflaging, or omitting conflicting information. Finally, a
research worker must write so clearly and in such detail that his
study may be replicated exactly. Therefore, an investigator owes
his reader a thorough demonstration of the fact that all
materials appearing in the "related literature" have been
selected through the use of appropriate criteria. He also owes
his reader evidence that the data and information compiled within
the study can withstand the rigorous tests applied by trained
research workers.
8 P. M. Symonds, "Common Faults in
Graduate Research in Education," Journal of Educational
Research, 27 (March 1934), pp. 481-492.